Volume 14, Issue 28 (12-2024)                   JRSM 2024, 14(28): 34-51 | Back to browse issues page

XML Persian Abstract Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Naseri A, Bahram A, Salehi H, Daneshfar A. The role of errorless and error full training on aiming task learning of normal teens and intellectual disability ones. JRSM 2024; 14 (28) :34-51
URL: http://jrsm.khu.ac.ir/article-1-2556-en.html
1- Assistant Professor, Farhangian University, Tehran, Iran. , Ali_naseri1356@yahoo.com
2- Professor, Kharazmi University, Tehran, Iran.
3- Associate Professor, University of Isfahan, Isfahan, Iran.
4- Associate Professor, Alzahra University, Tehran, Iran.
Abstract:   (5548 Views)
ABSTRACT
This study aimed to examine the role of working memory in the effectiveness of errorless protocols for facilitating implicit motor learning in adolescents with and without intellectual disabilities. A total of 20 adolescent boys with intellectual disabilities and 20 typically developing adolescent boys (mean age = 12.12 ± 0.80 years) voluntarily participated in the experiment. The participants with and without intellectual disabilities were separately assigned to either the errorless or errorful practice group based on their IQ scores. The task involved aiming at concentric targets using a mini-basketball. The errorless practice group practiced the task from near to far distances (3, 3.25, 3.5, and 3.75 meters), while the errorful practice group practiced from far to near distances. Implicit motor learning activation was assessed by comparing performances in dual-task conditions immediately after practice, as well as after 24 hours and one week. Additionally, working memory engagement during practice was evaluated. Results indicated that the errorless group, which required less working memory engagement during practice, showed significantly better performance in dual-task conditions compared to the errorful group (p < 0.05). The findings regarding the impact of practice errors on enhancing implicit motor learning in both adolescents with and without intellectual disabilities are crucial for developing motor skill acquisition strategies for adolescents, regardless of cognitive abilities. The results were consistent with the predictions of Adams' closed-loop theory and the reinvestment theory concerning the benefits of reducing errors in motor learning, but they were not aligned with Schmidt's schema theory.

 
Full-Text [PDF 1946 kb]   (34 Downloads)    
Type of Study: Research | Subject: motor behavior
Received: 2023/05/20 | Accepted: 2024/01/19 | ePublished ahead of print: 2024/01/19 | Published: 2024/12/30

References
1. Adams JA. A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1971;3:111-50. [DOI:10.1080/00222895.1971.10734898]
2. Fitts PM, Posner MI. Human performance. Oxford, England: Cole/Brooks; 1967.
3. Schmidt RA, Lee TD, Winstein C, Wulf G, Zelaznik HN. Motor learning concepts an research methods. In: Schmidt RA, Lee TD, Winstein C, Wulf G, Zelaznik HN, editors. Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. 6 ed. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2018. p. 283-302.
4. Elliott D, Bunn L. Motor disorders in children with intellectual disabilities. In: Dewey D, Tupper DE, editors. Developmental motor disorders: A neuropsychological perspective. New York London: THE GUILFORD PRESS; 2004. p. 137-51.
5. Vuijk PJ, Hartman E, Scherder E, Visscher C. Motor performance of children with mild intellectual disability and borderline intellectual functioning. Journal of intellectual disability research. 2010;54(11):955-65. [DOI:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01318.x]
6. Westendorp M, Houwen S, Hartman E, Visscher C. Are gross motor skills and sports participation related in children with intellectual disabilities? Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2011;32(3):1147-53. [DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.01.009]
7. Van der Molen M, Van Luit J, Van der Molen MW, Jongmans MJ. Everyday memory and working memory in adolescents with mild intellectual disability. American journal on intellectual and developmental disabilities. 2010;115(3):207-17. [DOI:10.1352/1944-7558-115.3.207]
8. Van der Molen M, Van Luit J, Jongmans M, Van der Molen M. Verbal working memory in children with mild intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2007;51(2):162-9. [DOI:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00863.x]
9. Henry L. Short-term memory coding in children with intellectual disabilities. American journal on mental retardation. 2008;113(3):187-200. [DOI:10.1352/0895-8017(2008)113[187:SMCICW]2.0.CO;2]
10. Schmidt RA. A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological review. 1975;82(4):225-60. [DOI:10.1037/h0076770]
11. Schmidt RA. The schema as a solution to some persistent problems in motor learning theory. Motor control: Issues and trends. 1976:41-65. [DOI:10.1016/B978-0-12-665950-4.50007-9]
12. Guadagnoli MA, Lee TD. Challenge point: A framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior. 2004;36(2):212-24. [DOI:10.3200/JMBR.36.2.212-224]
13. Masters, Maxwell. The theory of reinvestment. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2008;1(2):160-83. [DOI:10.1080/17509840802287218]
14. Masters RSW. Knowledge, knerves and know‐how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology. 1992;83(3):343-58. [DOI:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02446.x]
15. Hardy L, Mullen R, Jones G. Knowledge and conscious control of motor actions under stress. British Journal of Psychology. 1996;87:621-36. [DOI:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1996.tb02612.x]
16. Bright JEH, Freedman O. Differences between implicit and explicit acquisition of a complex motor skill under pressure: An examination of some evidence. British Journal of Psychology. 1998;89(2):249-63. [DOI:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1998.tb02683.x]
17. Maxwell JP, Masters RSW, Kerr E, Weedon E. The implicit benefit of learning without errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A. 2001;54(4):1049-68. [DOI:10.1080/713756014]
18. Maxwell JP, Masters RSW, Kerr E, Weedon E. The implicit benefit of learning without errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A. 2001;54(4):1049-68. [DOI:10.1080/713756014]
19. Kal E, Prosee R, Winters M, van der Kamp J. Does implicit motor learning lead to greater automatization of motor skills compared to explicit motor learning? A systematic review. PLoS One. 2018;13(9):e0203591. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0203591]
20. Masters RSW, Poolton JM. Advances in implicit motor learning. In: Hodges NJ, Williams AM, editors. Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory and practice. 2 ed. London & New York: Routledge; 2012. p. 59-76.
21. Sanli EA, Lee TD. What roles do errors serve in motor skill learning? An examination of two theoretical predictions. Journal of motor behavior. 2014;46(5):329-37. [DOI:10.1080/00222895.2014.913544]
22. Sanli EA, Lee TD. Nominal and functional task difficulty in skill acquisition: Effects on performance in two tests of transfer. Human Movement Science. 2015;41:218-29. [DOI:10.1016/j.humov.2015.03.006]
23. Salehi H, Mehrvarz M, Rafaei M. Errorless practice for facilitate implicit motor learning in children. Journal of Development & Motor Learning. 2018;10(3):367-84 (In Persian).
24. Van der Molen MJ, Van Luit JEH, Van der Molen MW, Jongmans MJ. Everyday memory and working memory in adolescents with mild intellectual disability. American Journal on Intellectual and Develop- mental Disabilities. 2010;115:207-17. [DOI:10.1352/1944-7558-115.3.207]
25. van Abswoude F, Santos-Vieira B, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B. The influence of errors during practice on motor learning in young individual with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2015;45-46:353-64. [DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2015.08.008]
26. Naseri A, Bahram A, Salehi H, Daneshvar a. The effects of errorless practice on learning an aiming skill in mentally retarded adolescents. Journal of Motor Learning and Movement. 2019;11(3):295-310.
27. Hartman E, Houwen S, Scherder E, Visscher C. On the relationship between motor performance and executive functioning in children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2010;54(5):468-77. [DOI:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01284.x]
28. Planinsec J, Pisot R. Motor coordination and intelligence level in adolescents. Adolescence. 2006;41:667-76.
29. Barnett LM, Van Beurden E, Morgan PJ, Brooks LO, Beard JR. Childhood motor skill proficiency as a predictor of adolescent physical activity. ournal of adolescent health. 2009;44(3):252-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.07.004]
30. Poolton JM, Masters RSW, Maxwell JP. The relationship between initial errorless learning conditions and subsequent performance. Human Movement Science. 2005;24(3):362-78. [DOI:10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.006]
31. Poolton JM, Masters RSW, Maxwell JP. Passing thoughts on the evolutionary stability of implicit motor behaviour: Performance retention under physiological fatigue. Consciousness and Cognition. 2007;16(2):456-68. [DOI:10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.008]

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Send email to the article author


Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

© 2025 CC BY-NC 4.0 | Research in Sport Management and Motor Behavior

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb